Language and Narrative Skills in Young Children with Special Health Care Needs

Language and Narrative Skills in Young Children with Special Health Care Needs

Problem Statement: Previous studies showed that children with special health care needs are at increased risk for future linguistic problems and deficits in narrative abilities. Despite this evidence of increased risk, there has been limited research on the impact of young children's health problems on specific aspects of linguistic development. Purpose of Study: This study examined the relationship between the complexity of health care needs and language and narrative skills in young children with special health care needs. Method: Ninety-nine children, ages 4 to 6 years, were classified using a rating system in which the number of organ systems affected and severity of complications determined the complexity of health care needs. Children were stratified by rating complexity, which resulted in three groups. While Group 1 included healthy children, Group 2 included children with less complex health care needs, and Group 3 included children with more complex health care needs. The Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence - Revised (WPPSI-R) and the Test of Early Language Development (TELD-III) were administered to each child in the study. Children also completed a story generation task and produced personal narratives in response to three conversation prompts. Children's narratives were evaluated on two dimensions: productivity (total number of words and C-units) and quality (where performance was rated in accordance with nine indicators of quality). Results: Groups with different levels of complexity of health care needs were compared on language and narrative measures, using gender and WPPSI-R scores as covariates. The results showed that children with more complex health care needs demonstrated greater weaknesses on expressive language and narrative tasks than children with less complex health care needs and those who were healthy. Conclusions: The findings support the contention that health issues are related to children's language status. The complexity of health care needs may hinder the development of language and narrative skills. The results of this study suggest that professionals in health care, speech and language, and education should be alerted to the potential connection between health and language. Yet efforts to identify language and narrative deficits early in this special population should lead to earlier interventions for children who are at risk for the development of later learning difficulties.

___

  • Bakeman, R. & Gottman, J. M. (1997). Observing interaction: An introduction to sequential analysis. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Barnes, M. & Dennis, M. (1998). Discourse after early-onset hydrocephalus: Core deficits in children of average intelligence. Brain and Language, 61, 309-334.
  • Boekaerts, M. & Roder, I. (1998). Stress, coping, and adjustment in children with a chronic disease: A review of the literature. Disability and Rehabilitation, 21, 311-337.
  • Bramlett, M. D., Read, D., Bethell, C., & Blumberg, S. J. (2009). Differentiating subgroups of children with special health care needs by health status and complexity of health care needs. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 13, 151- 163.
  • Bryan, T., Burstein, K., Chao, P. C., & Ergul, C. (2006). The relationship between health status, language development, and behavior in young children. Physical Disabilities: Education and Related Services, 24(2), 7-20.
  • Chapman, S. B., McKinnon, L., Levin, H. S., Song, J., Meier, M. C., & Chiu, S. (2001). Longitudinal outcome of verbal discourse in children with traumatic brain injury: Three year follow-up. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 16, 441- 455.
  • Day, A. (1997). Good dog Carl. New York, NY: Aladdin Paperback.
  • Fiestas, C. & Pena, E. (2004). Narrative discourse in bilingual children: Task and language effects. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 35, 155- 168.
  • Garrison, W. T. & McQuiston, S. (1989). Chronic illness during childhood and adolescence. Newbury Park, CA: Sage
  • Girolametto, L., Wiigs, M., Smyth, R., Weitzman, E., & Pearce, P. S. (2001). Children with a history of expressive vocabulary delay: Outcomes at 5 years of age. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 10, 358-370.
  • Green, S. B. & Salkind, N. J. (2005). Using SPSS for Windows and Macintosh: Analyzing and understanding data (4th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson.
  • Griffin, T. M., Hemphill, L., Camp, L., & Wolf, D. P. (2004). Oral discourse in the preschool years and later literacy skills. First Language, 24, 123-147.
  • Harkins, D. A., Koch, P. E., & Michel, G. F. (1994). Listening to maternal story-telling affects narrative skill of 5-year-old children. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 55, 247-257.
  • Hemphill, L., Uccelli, P., Winner, K., Chang, C., & Bellinger, D. (2002). Narrative discourse in young children with histories of early corrective heart surgery. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 318-331.
  • Hresco, W., Reid, K., & Hammill, D. D. (1999). Test of Early Language Development. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
  • Hunt, K. W. (1965). Grammatical structures written at three grade levels (Research Report No. 3). Champaign, IL: National Council of Teachers of English.
  • Kaderavek, J. & Justice, L. M. (2005). The effect of book genre in the repeated readings of mothers and their children with language impairment: A pilot investigation. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 21, 75-92.
  • Law, J. & Harris, F. (2000). The parameters of early language screening. Child: Care, Health and Development, 26, 83-89.
  • McCabe, A. & Rollins, P. R. (1994). Assessment of preschool narrative skills. American Journal of Speech Language Pathology, 3(1), 45-56. McPherson, M., Arango, P., Fox, H., Lauver, C., McManus, M., Newacheck, P. W., Perrin, J. M., Shonkoff, J. P., & Srtrickland, B. (1998). A new definition of children with special health care needs. Pediatrics, 102, 137-141.
  • Paul, R. & Hernandez, R. (1996). Narrative development on late talkers: Early school age. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 39, 1295-1304.
  • Peterson, C., Jesso, B., & McCabe, A. (1999). Encouraging narratives in preschoolers: An intervention study. Journal of Child Language, 26, 49-67.
  • Reilly, J. S., Bates, E. A., & Marchman, V. A. (1998). Narrative discourse in children with early focal brain injury. Brain and Language, 61, 335-375.
  • Scarborough, H. S. (2001). Connecting early language and literacy to later reading dis(abilities): Evidence, theory, and practice. In S. B. Neuman & D. K. Dickinson (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (pp. 213-245). New York, NY: Guilford.
  • Schoenbrodt, L., Kerins, M., & Gesell, J. (2003). Using narrative language intervention as a tool to increase communicative competence in Spanish-speaking children. Language, Culture and Curriculum, 16, 48-59.
  • Shiro, M. (1995). Focus on research: Venezuelan preschoolers' oral narrative abilities. Language Arts, 72, 528-537.
  • Stein, R. E. K., Westbrook, L. E., & Bauman, L. J. (1997). The questionnaire for identifying children with chronic conditions: A measure based on a noncategorical approach. Pediatrics, 99, 513-521.
  • Tu, H. & Cunningham, P. (2005). Public coverage provides vital safety net for children with special health care needs. Center for Studying Health System Change, 98, 1-4.
  • Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Revised. New York, NY: Psychological Corp.