Turkish validation of the maternal responsiveness global rating scale in slow-to-talk toddlers

Background. The relation between maternal responsiveness and language development has been shown in previous literature however it is difficult to evaluate responsiveness because of the difficulties involved when coding the specific patterns of parent-child interactions. The maternal responsiveness global rating scale is important because it requires less time and expertise from professionals and predicts the language outcome of children. The aim of the study was to adapt the Maternal Responsiveness Global Rating Scale into Turkish, thereby making it accessible to a variety of professionals, and creating a way to use this useful scale.Methods. Twenty-seven 18- to 42-month old children who had been admitted to the Developmental Pediatrics outpatient clinic with concerns of speech delay and had received a diagnosis of language disorder with the standardized language test were included in the study. The general development and language development of each participant was evaluated using Denver II, Bayley-third edition and Pre-school Language Scale-5. After the translation study of the Maternal Responsiveness Global Rating Scale, video collecting and rating procedures and finally reliability and validity analyses were implemented.Results. The results of this study demonstrated that the Turkish translation of the Maternal Responsiveness Global Rating Scale shows strong evidence of adequate reliability and validity and is a feasible tool to measure responsiveness in routine child health care practice for children with language delay. Conclusions. This in expensive, easy-to-use and reliable tool may be recommended in order to identify which slow-to-talk toddlers and their mothers need early intervention and may be used by community-based practitioners and researchers in Turkey to support language development during early intervention stages.

___

1.Reilly S, Wake M, Ukoumunne OC, et al. Predicting language outcomes at 4 years of age: findings from early language in Victoria study. Pediatrics 2010; 126: e1530-e1537.

2.Levickis P, Reilly S, Girolametto L, Ukoumunne OC, Wake M. Maternal behaviors promoting language acquisition in slow-to-talk toddlers: prospective community-based study. J Dev Behav Pediatr 2014; 35: 274-281.

3.Hudson S, Levickis P, Down K, Nicholls R, Wake M. Maternal responsiveness predicts child language at ages 3 and 4 in a community-based sample of slow-to-talk toddlers. Int J Lang Commun Disord 2015; 50: 136-142.

4.Owens RE. Language Disorders: A Functional Approach to Assessment and Intervention (5th ed). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon, 2010.

5.Topping K, Dekhinet R, Zeedyk S. Parent–infant interaction and children’s language development. Educ Psychol 2012; 33: 391-426.

6.Hebert H, Swank P, Smith K, Landry S. Maternal support for play and language across early childhood. Early Educ Dev 2004; 15: 93-116.

7.Brady N, Warren SF, Sterling A. Interventions aimed at improving child language by improving maternal responsivity. Int Rev Res Ment Retard 2009; 37: 333-357.

8.Aspland H, Gardner F. Observational measures of parent-child interaction: an introductory review. Child Adolesc Ment Health 2003; 8: 136-143.

9.Marfo K. Correlates of maternal directiveness with children who are developmentally delayed. Am J Orthopsychiatry 1992; 62: 219-233.

10.Bayoglu B, Unal O, Elibol F, Karabulut E, Innocenti MS. Turkish validation of the PICCOLO (parenting interactions with children: checklist of observations linked to outcomes). Infant Ment Health J 2013; 34: 330-338.

11.Durmazlar N, Öztürk C, Ural B, Karagaoglu E, Anlar B. Turkish children’s performance on Denver II: effect of sex and mother’s education. Dev Med Child Neurol 1998; 40: 411-416.

12.Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development (3rd ed). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment. 2006: DOI: 10.1177/0734282906297199.

13.Sahli AS, Belgin E. Adaptation, validity, and reliability of the Preschool Language Scale-Fifth Edition (PLS-5) in the Turkish context: the Turkish Preschool Language Scale-5 (TPLS-5). Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 2017; 98: 143-149.

14.Down K, Levickis P, Hudson S, Nicholls R, Wake M. Measuring maternal responsiveness in a community-based sample of slow-to-talk toddlers: a cross-sectional study. Child Care Health Dev 2014; 41: 329-333.

15.Roggman LA, Cook GA, Innocenti MS, Norman VJ, Christiansen K. Parenting interactions with children: checklist of observations linked to outcomes (PICCOLO) in diverse Ethnic Groups. Infant Ment Health J 2013; 34: 290-306.

16.Frankenburg WK, Dodds J, Archer P, Shapiro H, Bresnick B. The Denver II: a major revision and restandardization of the Denver Developmental Screening Test. Pediatrics 1992; 89: 91-97.

17.Hollingshead AB, Redlich FC. Social class and mental illness: a community study. Am J Public Health 2007; 97: 1756-1757.

18.Zhou XH, Obuchowski NA, McClish DK. Statistical Methods in Diagnostic Medicine (2nd ed). New York: John Wiley & Sons, 2002: 27-29.

19.Gardner F. Methodological issues in the direct observation of parent-child interaction: do observational findings reflect the natural behavior of participants? Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 2000; 3: 185-198.

20.Karaaslan Ö, Mahoney G. Mediational analyses of the effects of responsive teaching on the developmental functioning of preschool children with disabilities. J Early Interv 2015; 37: 286-299.

21.Landry SH, Smith KE, Swank PR, Assel MA, Vellet S. Does early responsive parenting have a special importance for children’s development or is consistency across early childhood necessary? Dev Psychol 2001; 37: 387-403.

22.McDuffie A, Yoder P. Types of parent verbal responsiveness that predict language in young children with autism spectrum disorder. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2010; 53: 1026-1039.

23.Kochanska G, Forman DR, Aksan N, Dunbar SB. Pathways to conscience: early mother-child mutually responsive orientation and children’s moral emotion, conduct, and cognition. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2005; 46: 19-34.

24.Mahoney G, Kim JM, Lin CS. Pivotal behavior model of developmental learning. Infant Young Children 2007; 20: 311-325.
Turkish Journal of Pediatrics-Cover
  • ISSN: 0041-4301
  • Yayın Aralığı: Yılda 6 Sayı
  • Başlangıç: 1958
  • Yayıncı: Hacettepe Üniversitesi Çocuk Sağlığı Enstitüsü Müdürlüğü
Sayıdaki Diğer Makaleler

Clinical characteristics of children with congenital anomalies of the kidney and urinary tract and predictive factors of chronic kidney disease

Pınar GÜR ÇETINKAYA, Bora GÜLHAN, Ali DÜZOVA, Nesrin BEŞBAŞ, Mutlu HAYRAN, Rezan TOPALOĞLU, Fatih ÖZALTIN

Carbapenem and colistin resistance in children with Enterobacteriaceae infections

Dinçer YILDIZDAŞ, Özlem Özgür GÜNDEŞLİOĞLU, Emine KOCABAŞ, Zeliha HAYTOĞLU, Derya ALABAZ, Özden ÖZGÜR HOROZ

Atypical presentation in patients with 17 α-hydroxylase deficiency caused by a deletion in the CYP17A1 gene: short stature

Semih BOLU, Recep ERÖZ, Mehmet TEKİN, Mustafa DOĞAN

Response to “Neonatal form of biotin-thiamine-responsive basal ganglia disease. Clues to diagnosis”

Aydan DEĞERLİYURT, Serdar CEYLANER

Rituximab-induced serum sickness and anaphylaxis in a child with nephrotic syndrome

Meral TORUN BAYRAM, Alper SOYLU, Salih KAVUKÇU

Are homozygous SLC19A3 deletions non-responsive to thiamine/biotin?

Josef FINSTERER

High-grade neuroepithelial tumor with medulloepithelioma-like areas out of the central nervous system in an infant with hemihypertrophy: a unique association

İbrahim KARNAK, Gökhan GEDİKOĞLU, Berna OĞUZ, Figen SÖYLEMEZOĞLU, Armita BAHRAMI, Jason CHIANG, Tezer KUTLUK

Gender-related differences in etiology of organic central precocious puberty

Alev ÖZÖN, Doğuş VURALLI, Ayfer ALİKAŞİFOĞLU, E. Nazlı GÖNÇ, Nurgün KANDEMİR, Kader K. OĞUZ

Neonatal outcomes of early- and late-onset preeclampsia

Melek BÜYÜKEREN, HASAN TOLGA ÇELİK, Gökçen ÖRGÜL, Şule YİĞİT, Mehmet Sinan BEKSAÇ, Murat YURDAKÖK

A rare cause of hepatomegaly and dyslipidemia: lysosomal acid lipase deficiency

Berrak BİLGİNER GÜRBÜZ, İlker GÜNEY, Okan DİLEK